LSE - Small Logo
LSE - Small Logo

Heather Kappes

April 12th, 2019

Understanding how we spend our money

0 comments

Estimated reading time: 5 minutes

Heather Kappes

April 12th, 2019

Understanding how we spend our money

0 comments

Estimated reading time: 5 minutes

Heather Kappes, Assistant Professor of Marketing at LSE’s Department of Management, spent six weeks during January and February 2019 doing research as part of Live Science at the Science Museum in London. In a time where money is still a taboo topic, Heather wanted to explore people’s attitudes to spending. Here’s what she found out…

My project, billed as ‘The Science of Spending’, was made possible by funding from LSE’s Knowledge Exchange and Impact (KEI) fund, and run with help from Rebecca Campbell and five fantastic students from the MSc Marketing programme.

More than 2,000 museum visitors completed some part of the research. They played ‘The Game of Life’, where they were given some ‘gold’ and had to decide how much to spend each month over 12 months. The basic screen looked like this:

Here are a few of the things we found out, using the game:

  1. People with predictable patterns getting gold did best.

We created three different income streams by varying the months in which people were given gold.

The different income streams offered.

People who were randomly assigned to the early income stream got most of their gold in the early months. They earned 753 points on average.

People with the late income stream got most of their gold in the ending months. They earned 762 points on average.

Finally, a third group of players had a predictable income stream — a month off, a month on. Many people in this version actually told us that they figured it out and they knew that money was coming every second month. In this case they were able to use their money more advantageously. They scored 855 points on average, which is more than participants with the irregular early or late income streams.

There has been a lot of attention recently given to the challenges of coping with unpredictable income, like what people experience when they have a zero-hours contract. Our research reinforces the argument that budgeting can be much easier — and people can be more successful — when income is predictable.

  1. People did worse when they were in a group with high-scoring others.
The leaderboard presented to participants.

Those who took part in the first two weeks of the study didn’t see anyone else’s score (unless they were peeking at a friend’s screen…). At the end of the first two weeks, we put people into groups to play the game so you saw how your score compared to seven other group members.  The group all had the same initial endowment (but may have had different income streams). In these versions, participants saw a leaderboard.

We built three different types of groups by drawing either mostly very high-scoring players, mostly low-scoring players, or a range of players (an ‘average’ group). Those who played alone earned the highest scores on average (823 points) compared to those who played against bad groups (798), average groups (764), or very good groups (700). If the low scores are because people spent too much, then these results are similar to what other researchers have found; the pressure to move up in a group makes many people spend more than they should, and their final score suffers.

  1. The game does seem to reflect people’s real financial decisions.

One of our goals with this project was to see whether the way people play in the game reflected the way they manage their money in real life. We did this by including some questionnaires after the game that measured different aspects of how people manage their own money. With visitors over 16, for instance, we asked: “Imagine you had an unexpected expense of £1,500. How likely is it that you could pay this amount, in full and on-time, without borrowing or using credit?” Taking into consideration all the variations above, people who scored higher in the game said this was more likely.

People who scored high in the game also reported being less stressed about their own money currently (although not any more likely to expect that they’d be financially secure in the future!). These results suggest there is something about this game that measures the way people spend their money, and so the game may be useful for future research. For example, if an employer wanted to see whether the financial training they give to employees was effective, they could use this game to get a fast answer, instead of waiting months to see how the employees’ spending might change. We’ll have to do some more research to see exactly how this could work, but we’re excited about the possibility.

  1. Other questionnaire measures also related to game scores.

We gave some questions that measure numeracy, which are drawn from those that the Money Advice Service etc. have used. Not all participants saw these questions or chose to answer them — many groaned about seeing maths! But among the nearly 1,000 who did, there was a strong relationship between numeracy scores (getting more questions right) and scoring highly in the game. You can test your numeracy here and work on improving it here, if you’re interested.

A participant playing the game. Photo courtesy of The Science Museum.
  1. A couple of final points.

Visitors from other countries (of which there was 569) did not perform differently on average than those from the UK (1,451 of them) and there was no difference between men and women. People who reported being more materialistic scored lower at the game, but this was only true for adults and not for children. It might be because the children’s questions are different, or because some children didn’t really understand well how to play the game. When people don’t understand how to play, there is more ‘noise’ variation (differences that are not meaningful), which makes it harder to see effects. (Do keep in mind that finding a relationship between two measures, like between materialism and game scores, certainly doesn’t mean one of these things causes the other one).

We had a lot of young participants, especially during the half-term week. We asked those who were under 16 years old: “How confident do you feel managing your money?” The good news is that most felt very confident; almost all of the answers were between 5 and 10 on the 10-point scale. The possibly bad news is that these answers were unrelated to scores in the game, so some young people (and some adults) may be more confident than they should be.

In conclusion…

We’ll be looking further into this data to learn more about spending in the coming months. If you’re interested in playing the game in the meantime, you can play it here.

This project was motivated in part by the fact that money is a taboo topic; most of us don’t feel very comfortable talking about it, and this makes it hard to learn. I hope playing the game helps make it easier to talk about money, so please do feel free to share it with anyone that might be interested.

Notes:

About the author

Heather Kappes

Assistant Professor of Marketing at LSE's Department of Management

Posted In: Research Diaries

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *